quinta-feira, 23 de julho de 2020

2015 Déjà vécu: When déjà vu becomes your reality



One drab afternoon a few years ago something very unusual happened to me.
I was lounging under a tree in a packed east London park when I experienced a sudden feeling of vertigo, followed immediately by an overwhelming and intense sense of familiarity.
The people around me vanished and I found myself lying on a tartan picnic blanket amid a field of high golden wheat. The memory was rich and detailed. I could hear the sway of the wheat ears as a gentle breeze brushed through them. I felt warm sunlight on the back of my neck and watched as birds wheeled and floated above me.
It was a pleasant and extremely vivid recollection. The problem was that it never actually happened. What I was experiencing was an extreme form of a very common mental illusion: déjà vu.
Living with persistent déjà vu
For the past five years I have been suffering epileptic seizures resulting from the growth and eventual removal of a lemon-sized tumour from the right-hand side of my brain. Before my diagnosis I appeared fit and healthy: I was in my mid-30s and displayed absolutely no symptoms. Until, that is, the afternoon that I woke up on the kitchen floor with two black eyes after suffering my first recorded seizure.


Seizures, or fits, occur after an unanticipated electrical discharge in the brain. They are usually preceded by something called an 'aura', a sort of minor foreshock lasting anything up to a couple of minutes before the main event begins. The nature of this aura differs greatly from patient to patient. Some people experience synaesthesia, extreme euphoria and even orgasm at the onset of a seizure. My own aren't nearly as exciting-sounding, being distinguished by sudden shifts in perspective, a rapidly increased heart rate, anxiety, and the occasional auditory hallucination.
By far the most significant trait of my aura is the striking sense of having lived through that precise moment before at some point in the past -- even though I never have. During my most intense seizures, and for a week or so afterwards, this feeling of precognition becomes so pervasive that I routinely struggle to discern the difference between lived events and dreams, between memories, hallucinations and the products of my imagination.
I don't remember déjà vu happening with any kind of regularity before the onset of my epilepsy. Now it occurs with varying degrees of magnitude up to ten times a day, whether as part of a seizure or not. I can find no pattern to explain when or why these episodes manifest themselves, only that they usually last for the length of a pulse before vanishing.
Many of the estimated 50 million people in the world with epilepsy experience long-term memory decline and psychiatric problems. And it's hard for me not to worry whether the blurring of fact and fiction that I experience might one day engender a kind of mania.
Exploring the déjà experience
Taken from the French for 'already seen', déjà vu is one of a group of related quirks of memory. Research from 50 different surveys suggests that around two-thirds of healthy people have experienced déjà vu at one time or another. For the majority, it is dismissed as a curiosity or a mildly interesting cognitive illusion.


While déjà vu is instantaneous and fleeting, déjà vécu (already lived) is far more troubling. Unlike déjà vu, déjà vécu involves the sensation that a whole sequence of events has been lived through before. What's more, it lacks both the startling aspect and instantly dismissible quality of déjà vu.
A defining feature of the normal déjà vu experience is the ability to discern that it isn't real. On encountering déjà vu, the brain runs a sort of sense check, searching for objective evidence of the prior experience and then disregarding it as the illusion that it is. People with déjà vécu have been known to lose this ability completely.
Professor Chris Moulin, one of the foremost experts on the déjà experience, describes a patient he encountered while working at a memory clinic at a hospital in Bath, England. In 2000, Moulin received a letter from a local GP referring an 80-year-old former engineer known as AKP. As a result of gradual brain-cell death caused by dementia, AKP was now suffering from chronic and perpetual déjà vu: déjà vécu.
AKP claimed that he had given up watching television or reading the newspaper because he knew what was about to happen. "His wife said that he was someone who felt as though everything in his life had happened before," says Moulin, now at the Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition CNRS in Grenoble. AKP was resistant to the idea of visiting the clinic because he felt as though he'd already been there, despite the fact that he never had. On being introduced to Moulin for the first time, the man even claimed to be able to give specific details of occasions that they had met before.


AKP did retain some self-awareness. "His wife would ask him how he could know what would happen in a television programme if he'd never seen it before," says Moulin, "to which he would respond, 'How would I know? I have a memory problem.'"
On that day in the park, my vision of the picnic blanket and the wheat field disappeared when a paramedic began to shake my shoulder. Despite the fact that my memories had been hallucinations, they still felt as valid as any truly autobiographical memory. Moulin classes this as a form of déjà experience in which an image is somehow imbued with a sense of reality.
"Our feeling is that déjà vu is caused by a sense of familiarity," he says. "Rather than just feeling like something has a feeling of 'pastness' about it, something comes to mind that has a phenomenological characteristic, so that it appears to be a real reminiscence."
After his first encounter with AKP, Moulin began to become interested in the causes of déjà vu and how subjective feelings can interfere with day-to-day memory processes. Discovering that there was very little credible literature describing the causes of déjà vu, Moulin and colleagues at the Language and Memory Lab at the Institute of Psychological Sciences, University of Leeds, began to study epileptics and other sufferers of profound memory defects in order to draw conclusions about déjà experiences in the healthy brain and explore what déjà vu means for the workings of consciousness generally.
They were faced with an immediate problem: déjà vu experiences can be so transitory and short-lived that they are almost impossible to recreate in clinical conditions. The job that they faced, then, was one of trying to catch lightning in a bottle.
The causes of déjà vu
In his book The Déjà Vu Experience, Professor Alan S Brown offers 30 different explanations for déjà vu. According to him, any one alone may be enough to trigger a déjà experience. As well as a biological dysfunction like epilepsy, Brown writes that stress or tiredness could cause déjà vu.
Brown is also a proponent of what is called the divided perception theory. First described in the 1930s by Dr Edward Bradford Titchener, divided perception refers to the times when the brain isn't quite paying enough attention to its surroundings. Titchener used the example of a person about to cross a busy street before being distracted by a shop window display. "As you cross," he wrote, "you think, 'Why, I crossed this street just now'; your nervous system has severed two phases of a single experience, and the latter appears as a repetition of the earlier."
For much of the last century this idea was accepted as a plausible trigger of déjà vu. Another common explanation was one offered by a doctor working at the Boston veterans' hospital. In 1963 Robert Efron suggested that déjà vu could be caused by a sort of processing error: he believed that brains were responsible for assimilating events through the temporal lobe before then adding a sort of timestamp to them to determine when they happened.
Efron saw déjà vu as resulting from the lag between seeing and adding that timestamp: if the process took too long, the brain would think that an event had already happened.
But Alan Brown and Chris Moulin both agree that the way that the hippocampus indexes memories by cross-referencing them according to familiarity is a more likely cause of déjà vu.
"My belief is that a per-seizure déjà vu experience is triggered by spontaneous activity in that area of the brain that handles familiarity evaluations," says Brown. Probably, he says, in the area surrounding the hippocampus, and most likely on the right side of the brain. The precise point at which I have a lemon-shaped hole.
Brown suggests that déjà vu happens to healthy people only a few times a year at most, but can be stimulated by environmental factors. "People experience it mainly when they are indoors," he says, "doing leisure activities or relaxing, and in the company of friends; fatigue or stress frequently accompany the illusion." He says that déjà vu is relatively brief (10 to 30 seconds), and is more frequent in the evening than in the morning, and on the weekend than on weekdays.
Some researchers claim a connection between the ability to remember dreams and the likelihood of experiencing déjà vu. In his work, Brown suggests that although déjà vu occurs equally in women and men, it is more common in younger people, those that are well-travelled, earn higher incomes and whose political and social outlooks are more aligned to the liberal.
"There are some plausible explanations for this," he tells me. "People who travel more have more opportunities to encounter a new setting that they may find strangely familiar. People with liberal beliefs may be more likely to admit to having unusual mental experiences and willing to figure them out. A conservative mindset would likely avoid admitting to having strange mental events, as they might be seen as a sign that they are unstable.
"The age issue is a puzzle because memory usually gets more quirky as we age, rather than the other way around. I would guess that young people are more open to experiences and more in touch with unusual mental happenings."
One of the first comprehensive studies of déjà vu was conducted in the 1940s by a New York undergraduate student called Morton Leeds. Leeds kept an extraordinarily detailed diary of his frequent déjà experiences, noting 144 episodes over the course of a year. One of these episodes, he wrote, was "so strong that it almost nauseated me".
Following my most recent seizures I've experienced something similar. The shock of repeated déjà vu isn't physical, necessarily, but instead causes a kind of psychic pain that can feel physically sickening. Dream images suddenly interrupt normal thoughts. Conversations seem to have already taken place. Even banal things like making a cup of tea or reading a particular newspaper headline seem familiar. It feels occasionally like I'm flicking through a photo album containing nothing but the same picture reproduced endlessly.
Some of these sensations are easier to dismiss than others. Coming closer to finding an answer to what causes déjà vu also means approaching a kind of resolution for my more persistent déjà episodes, the ones that are the hardest of all to live with.
The night before completing this piece I had another seizure. The deadline had clearly been on my mind, as I suddenly had an intense memory of sitting down to write these closing sentences. When I regained my composure enough to read the finished article the next day, there was nothing here but blank space. It was another illusion. Now I'm actually typing this conclusion. It is, to borrow a famous solecism, like déjà vu all over again.
https://blogger.googleusercontent.com/img/proxy/AVvXsEgcaSCoOIh5oeEyEcWzfLyRE4vkzonHHTT6_QoQyFNjA0ibmzszBcB9Y8dABFBE6ZT5eH-ATZ3Onw39P5vARjzftJGo1qQHEAWeoYVDAjX2IUI9U_MVu9uzubOOxhID2Zs8xhIEwBZNYm4e1wh8f6f0NgOPFtWCo27u0zHi5ww=s0-d-e1-ft
This is an edited version of an article that was first published by Wellcome on Mosaic. It is republished here under a Creative Commons licence.
Copyright 2015 The Wellcome Trust. Some rights reserved.

quarta-feira, 22 de julho de 2020

STILL THINK RACISM DOESN’T EXIST IN CANADA!?!?

terça-feira, 21 de julho de 2020

There are 193 species of frogs and toads

There are 193 species of frogs and toads in Costa Rica but most people don't really pay attention to them. Nevertheless, they are very interesting animals. Did you know that they absorb water through their skin so they don't need to drink or that frog bones form a new ring every year when the frog is hibernating, just like trees do? Learn more about the fascinating wildlife of Costa Rica in our Nature Guide:
Costa Rica Nature https://hubs.ly/H0f7Q6R0

Updated 1804 GMT (0204 HKT) September 19, 2018

Millions of chickens and thousands of pigs died in Florence in North Carolina


North Carolina's poultry farms suffered significant losses in Hurricane Florence, while flooding from the storm damaged crops in South Carolina, industry officials said this week.
Some 3.4 million poultry birds were killed in North Carolina, according to the state Department of Agriculture & Consumer Services. That exceeds poultry losses two years ago in Hurricane Matthew, it said.
The state last year tallied about 830 million head of broiler chickens, plus 32 million head of turkeys, a federal inventory shows.
    Sanderson Farms, one of the largest poultry producers in North Carolina, lost 1.7 million chickens on its farms, and 60 of its 880 production chicken broiler houses have flooded, the company said in a news release.
    In addition, about 30 Sanderson farms that together house more than 6 million chickens around hard-hit Lumberton, North Carolina, remain isolated by floodwater and out of reach of feed trucks, Sanderson said, noting that the number of dead chickens could increase.
    Sanderson Farms has approximately 20 million head of chickens and doesn't expect this loss to affect operations, it said.
    Meantime, swine losses in North Carolina are estimated at 5,500, state veterinarian Dr. Doug Meckes said. There were 9 million hogs in the state last year, the inventory shows.

    South Carolina agriculture also hit

    In South Carolina, floodwaters heavily affected some crops, Agriculture Commissioner Hugh Weathers told Southern Farm Network.
    Commodities hardest hit, according to Weathers, are:
    • Cotton: These farmers will suffer the greatest impact because of timing. Heavy rain and wind soaked the cotton and "literally knocked it out of the bolls."
    • Peanuts: Peanuts don't suffer much from wind because they grow underground, but water on fields is a problem. Peanuts are now ready to be dug, then have to lay on the fields to dry before harvest. If the ground stays wet, that's tough.
      • Hemp: The plants are big, and the stems are small, so they've "literally blown over."
      • Livestock: The main concern is finding a way to dispatch feed trucks in flooded areas.

      This Fart Guide Is A Must Read For Every Couple That Sleeps In The Same Bed

      During the first phase of most romantic relationships, we try to look as desirable as possible, even if that means ignoring some of our bodily urges. Like, letting out a fart, for example. Sooner or later, however, most couples realize that there’s no need to blow against the wind and deny their biology, sharing their first fart. Weng Chen, the illustrator behind The Adventures of Messy Cow, thinks it’s perfectly normal, too. To highlight this, she has created a humorous guide on how to fart for everyone who is sharing a bed with their significant other, and these funny comics might be more useful than you’d think.
      “I ate all kinds of food and often ran into this problem,” Chen told Bored Panda. “I was curious how other people handled the farting in public situation, but it’s a hard topic to bring up in casual conversations. So I decided to make a webcomic about it and <...> was happily surprised by how many people were open to this discussion.”
      “Some couples can fart comfortably in front of each other after a week, some won’t do that after 50 years of long term relationships,” she added. “I think you should care about how it affects your relationship because you don’t want to make your partner dislike you because of smells and fart sounds, and it’s important you keep liking each other in a long-term relationship. I recommend using my funny comic strips as a guide and test it step by step. If your partner showed disgust at some point, don’t proceed further down the list.”
      Cartoonist Weng is Chinese, but she’s fluent in English and visual language, too. She started drawing manga at a very young age and has been creating comics on-and-off since. In late 2016, she started the Messy Cow series.

      The No-Fluff F.A.S.T.™Method To Profitable Launches Using Facebook + Instagram Ads ​That Create Nonstop Sales

      The No-Fluff F.A.S.T.Method 
      To Profitable Launches
      Using Facebook + Instagram Ads 
      That Create Nonstop Sales 

      Bliss is not Found in Faithfulness to Forms, But in Liberation From Them

      MythBlast | Bliss is not Found in Faithfulness to Forms, But in Liberation From Them

      I’ve been thinking a lot about this month’s theme at JCF, the theme of independence, reflecting on Joseph Campbell’s often bold, independent nature, and what it means to become an independent human being. As Campbell describes in Pathways to Bliss, releasing this month as an eBook, independence is a difficult achievement for humans:
      The first fact that distinguishes the human species from all others is that we are born too soon. We arrive, incapable of taking care of ourselves for something like fifteen years. Puberty doesn’t come along for twelve years or more, and physical maturity doesn’t arrive until our early twenties. During the greater part of this long arc of life, the individual is in a psychological situation of dependency. We are trained, as children, so that every stimulus, every experience, leads us simply to react, “Who will help me?” (Pathways to Bliss, p 11)
      The reflexive, human reaction to novelty and fear, to the unfamiliar or unknown is to ask, at least initially, “Who will help me?” Of course, the slowly dawning realization occurs that it is primarily oneself upon which one must rely, and this is the beginning of maturity. In our maturity others may still point the way or render aid, they may still give comfort, but ultimately one understands that one has the solitary, frequently heavy, responsibility to reach out, to investigate, or to seek counsel, to deal with and navigate life’s challenges. It’s made all the more difficult because as a rule, others no longer magically appear, as one’s parents may once have—unbidden, and just when they were most needed. This is fundamentally how one grows and matures, this is how one becomes an independent person.
      Such a move towards maturity and independence is equally important in relationship to our myths as well. Generally speaking, people believe that the objects of myth are somewhere “out there,” either in this world or in some adjacent, perhaps ancient, conventionally unapproachable or misapprehended realm. “Now,” Campbell writes, “it’s a basic mythological principle, I would say, that what is referred to in mythology as ‘the other world’ is really (in psychological terms) ‘the inner world.’ And what is spoken of as ‘future’ is ‘now (Ibid, 18).’” Later, on page 42 he notes, “[…] the incarnation—the avatar—is merely the model through which you find this miracle in yourself.” Ideally, one works to destroy the notion that the objects of mythic awe are somewhere out there, separate from oneself, while simultaneously discovering that one is, in fact, the thing one is searching for. It’s a bit of a paradox, isn’t it? We unconsciously create the objects of myth, project them out into the world so that they seem separate from ourselves, and yet in some sense the objects of myth were there all along (within and without) waiting to be created and become realized!
      The revolutionary change from dependence to independence in mythical thought is a substantive change from an orientation of mere “relationship” to an orientation of “use.” One shifts from a relational modality which simply places one in relationship to something “other” and “out there,” to a perspective of use which gives the objects of myth energy, gravity, and consequence within; one might say that the objects of myth are present-to-hand or ready-to-hand. Such a perspectival shift requires that the mythic idea, concept, or symbol experienced as “other” be destroyed in order to realize that one possesses these energies oneself. Destruction of the mythic symbols existing out there in “the other world,” leads to the discovery of the symbol’s deeper reality living within. By destroying the objects of myth through the recognition of them as projections or fantasies, the reality of what they represent may be directly experienced in the inner world. The iconoclastic move, the smashing of the image, results in the creation of a new space, a new reality; no longer a potential space, but an area of actual experience. This is why Campbell can say, responding to Bill Moyers’ statement about him being a man of faith, “No, I don’t have to have faith, I have experience (The Power of Myth, 208).”
      The importance of this willing destruction cannot be overstated. It represents the birth into a living reality of what was formerly a simplistic, dualistic concept or heuristic device; this move allows one to experience in a given moment, at first hand, a transcendent reality of bliss, not merely the products of projective mental processes. It enables myth to be used.
      Thanks for reading,
      We are celebrating not only independence this month, but the release of Campbell’s work Pathways to Bliss: Mythology and Personal Transformation in new formats. In addition to finding it as a paperback, you now can listen to it as an audio book, and soon you will be able to download it as a Kindle eBook.
      Best regards,